External eating can be defined as
eating in response to external food-related cues, such as the sight, smell and
taste of food, regardless of physical need. External eating has been linked to
overeating and it is considered a highly problemmatic eating behaviour due to
its association with higher body weights, more unhealthy food intake and
increased risk of relapse in eating disorders and obesity.
External eating has been
associated with increased BMI in a healthy weight sample. Furthermore, it is
positively correlated with increased BMI in children and
adults. External eating was found to be
extremely associated with fat intake than to carbohydrate intake.
External eating is positively correlated
with laboratory based food intake in adolescent girls and candy consumption in
children in experimental studies. It is
also associated with unhealthy
snack food intake in healthy weight women according to experimental studies .
Results of the researchs also reported that external eating is linked to
increased self-reported energy intake over three days and one month in healthy
weight women. In a current study which has only women participants, external
eating was associated with the intake of sweet food, rather than savoury food
during the taste tests.
External eating behaviour was
found to be associated with increased impulsivity especially in overweight and
obese individuals. It was suggested that environmental cues affect eating
behaviours of obese people more than non-obese people. However, more recent
studies revealed that all weight classes can be influenced by environment.
External eating correspond to a
relative insensitivity to internal hunger and satiety signals such as emotional
eating. Externality theory focuses on the external environment such as food
cues as a determinant of eating behaviour. Elevated responsiveness to food
related cues in the immediate environment cause overeating of external eater.
There is also a further difference of external eating from emotional eating.
This difference has been considered as an evolutionary adaptive response that
has been related to Neel’s thrifty-genotype concept. This concept suggests that
evolution has favored genetic adaptations that allow humans to survive during
periods of food shortages, including adaptations that allow them to overeat in
times of food surplus (whenever external food cues are present in the
environment) and rapidly develop fat on their bodies.
There are some interventions to
struggle with external eating. Cognitive control training is one of them. This
training strengths inhibitory control for reducing both attentional and motor
impulsivity. It is found that using general inhibitory control training (i.e.,
repeatedly inhibiting responses to stimuli unrelated to food) reduced unhealthy
food intake on a subsequent taste test. Others have used specific inhibitory
control training that focuses on food stimuli and found that increased
inhibitory control for chocolate cues as well as food in general can reduce
unhealthy food intake. In addition, increasing inhibitory control for unhealthy
food resulted in weight loss among dieters with high BMI.
Another intervention is food-cue
reactivity training. These intervention makes foods less tempting to external
eaters by reducing the saliency of attractive food cues. This method was found
effective at reducing cue reactivity for
unhealthy foods such as chocolate. This
particular intervention may be useful at reducing unhealthy food intake among
individuals with problematic eating behaviour, specifically those with a
pronounced external eating style.
No comments:
Post a Comment